The Unconscious Mind: A Journey from Philosophy to Neuroscience and Therapy
- Alberto Imbrogno
- 6 days ago
- 7 min read
The unconscious mind has long fascinated philosophers, clinicians, and neuroscientists. As a construct, it challenges the assumption that our behavior is fully guided by conscious, rational thought. Whether we're talking about sudden insights, emotional triggers we don't understand, or decisions we "feel" before we think, the unconscious remains central to understanding human cognition and mental health.

Philosophical and Psychological Origins of the Unconscious
The concept of the unconscious predates modern psychology. In ancient Greek philosophy, Plato suggested that humans have innate knowledge that can be "recollected" rather than learned — a rudimentary form of what would later be called unconscious content (Plato, trans. 1961). In the late 18th and 19th centuries, German Romantic philosophers laid the groundwork for a more formalized understanding. Friedrich Schelling introduced the unconscious into metaphysics, proposing that parts of the psyche were inaccessible to conscious introspection. Eduard von Hartmann (1869), in his book Philosophy of the Unconscious, argued for a teleological unconscious that guided both individual and cultural evolution.
The unconscious entered psychology in the 19th century through the work of William Benjamin Carpenter, who described unconscious mental activity as part of adaptive functioning (Merikle & Daneman, 1998). His “adaptive unconscious” was a system that filtered information and influenced decision-making without conscious mediation.
However, the person most credited with operationalizing the unconscious is Sigmund Freud. In his seminal work The Interpretation of Dreams (1900/1953), Freud conceptualized the unconscious as a repository for repressed desires, trauma, and unfulfilled drives — primarily of a sexual and aggressive nature. Freud’s structural model (id, ego, superego) was built around conflict and repression, with dreams, slips of the tongue, and neurotic symptoms providing indirect access to unconscious content (Freud, 1900/1953).
Carl Jung (1936) expanded on Freud’s ideas by introducing the collective unconscious — a level of unconscious shared among all humans, populated by universal archetypes such as the Mother, Hero, and Shadow. While Jung maintained that dreams and symbols could reveal unconscious content, he saw the unconscious as a source of spiritual insight and growth, not merely repression.
Contemporary Understanding of the Unconscious
With the rise of behaviorism in the early 20th century, the unconscious was temporarily dismissed as unobservable. However, the cognitive revolution in the 1950s and 1960s revived interest in internal mental states, albeit through the lens of empirical testing.
Today, the unconscious is no longer seen solely as repressed memory or instinct. Instead, it refers to a wide range of nonconscious cognitive processes — including automatic perception, implicit memory, emotional processing, and decision-making — that occur outside of awareness (Kihlstrom, 2007). These processes are adaptive, fast, and often efficient.
For example, implicit bias research shows that individuals can unknowingly associate negative or positive traits with certain groups (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). Priming studies reveal that exposure to certain stimuli can shape behavior in measurable ways, even if the person is unaware of the stimulus (Dehaene et al., 2006). The unconscious is also implicated in affective forecasting, moral decision-making, and creativity (Kounios & Beeman, 2014).
Research in Psychology and Neuroscience

Implicit Learning and Subliminal Perception
Early research on unconscious processing relied on subliminal priming. These studies typically present stimuli too briefly to be consciously perceived and then assess their impact on subsequent tasks. While initial results were promising, more recent meta-analyses highlight the methodological fragility of these studies. A major re-analysis by Meyen et al. (2020) showed that many unconscious priming effects may have been overestimated due to statistical errors.
Still, implicit memory — learning that occurs without conscious awareness — is widely accepted. For example, people can be exposed to a list of words and later complete word stems with those words, even if they don’t remember seeing them (Roediger & McDermott, 1995).
Decoded Neurofeedback and the Unconscious
Recent neuroscience has pushed the boundary even further. Cortese et al. (2020) demonstrated that participants could learn to reinforce specific brain states associated with decision accuracy — without knowing the pattern or rule. Using decoded neurofeedback (DecNef), participants received a reward when their brain entered a pre-identified neural configuration. Although they were unaware of what was being reinforced, their accuracy improved, and their confidence ratings tracked their performance. This shows that complex, unconscious learning is possible, and that metacognition (confidence) can operate even without explicit knowledge.
Consciousness and the Dynamic Brain
Studies on sleep, anesthesia, and disorders of consciousness show how consciousness — and by contrast unconsciousness — arises from dynamic brain networks. Morena del Pozo et al. (2020) found that unconscious states (e.g., deep sleep or under propofol) significantly reduce the brain’s modular flexibility. In other words, the richness and variability of brain activity — particularly in the default mode network — collapses during unconscious states, supporting theories that consciousness depends on dynamic information integration.
Strengths and Limitations of Current Evidence
Strengths:
Implicit learning and memory are robust and reproducible in cognitive psychology (Kihlstrom, 2007).
DecNef offers a novel method to causally demonstrate unconscious learning (Cortese et al., 2020).
Neuroscience provides observable, dynamic patterns differentiating conscious and unconscious states (Morena del Pozo et al., 2020).
Limitations:
Subliminal priming often fails in replication studies or depends heavily on experimental conditions (Meyen et al., 2020; Weingarten et al., 2016).
Many unconscious processes cannot be neatly isolated, leading to problems in operational definitions.
Conceptual challenges remain: Is non-reportable always nonconscious? What counts as unconscious — absence of awareness, memory, or intention?
Case Scenario 1: Integrated Therapy in Practice
“Anna,” a 32-year-old graphic designer, presents with panic attacks and a persistent sense of guilt. In therapy, early sessions reveal intense shame stemming from a controlling parental relationship. Through dream work and free association, Anna uncovers images of abandonment and helplessness — consistent with classical psychoanalytic models of repression (Freud, 1900/1953).
However, sessions also use CBT techniques. Anna tracks her negative automatic thoughts — like “I’m not enough” — which she now links to specific triggers. A neurofeedback tool helps her recognize rising physiological arousal associated with early panic — often before she’s even aware of distress.
Over months, Anna learns to “catch” these unconscious reactions earlier, increasing her sense of emotional mastery. This integrative approach, blending psychodynamic exploration with real-time biofeedback and cognitive restructuring, allows unconscious material to surface and be reprocessed both insightfully and behaviorally.
Case Scenario 2: DecNef and Experimental Psychology
“Mark,” a 25-year-old university student, volunteers for a study using DecNef. Researchers record his brain’s fMRI data while he performs a perceptual decision task. A machine-learning decoder identifies the neural pattern associated with correct choices — but Mark is never told this.
During the experiment, whenever his brain “accidentally” enters that pattern, he receives a small reward. Over time, he begins to show better-than-chance performance, even though he insists he doesn’t know the rule. Interestingly, his subjective confidence grows — indicating metacognitive tracking of unconscious learning (Cortese et al., 2020).
This case illustrates the power of nonconscious learning and supports models of unconscious metacognition. Such research could eventually inform interventions for stroke rehabilitation or conditions where patients cannot verbally engage with feedback.

Future Directions
Neuropsychoanalysis and Brain-Based Psychotherapy: Bridging psychodynamic theories with neuroscience (e.g., understanding repression through connectivity models) may lead to new integrated therapies (Solms, 2018).
AI-Inspired Modeling: Understanding unconscious inference and creativity could help build AI systems that emulate aspects of intuitive and non-symbolic processing.
Ethics and Regulation: As DecNef and similar tools become more accessible, ethical questions around consent, autonomy, and manipulation must be addressed.
Consciousness Research: Tools that track neural complexity could help clinicians distinguish between coma, vegetative states, and covert awareness — improving end-of-life decisions and rehabilitation strategies.
Conclusion
The unconscious is not a dusty Freudian relic but a living, dynamic domain that spans perception, memory, affect, and cognition. While modern psychology has corrected and refined early theories, it has also vindicated some of Freud’s insights — particularly regarding the limits of conscious control.
Today, a wealth of experimental evidence shows that our thoughts, choices, and emotions are shaped by processes beneath the surface. As neuroscience, AI, and clinical psychology continue to merge, understanding the unconscious may be one of the most transformative frontiers for both healing and innovation.
References
Cortese, A., Lau, H., & Kawato, M. (2020). Unconscious reinforcement learning of hidden brain states supported by confidence. Nature Communications, 11, Article 4429. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-17828-8
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007
Freud, S. (1953). The interpretation of dreams (J. Strachey, Trans.). Basic Books. (Original work published 1900)
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4
Jung, C. G. (1959). The concept of the collective unconscious. In H. Read, M. Fordham, G. Adler, & W. McGuire (Eds.), The collected works of C. G. Jung (R. F. C. Hull, Trans.; Vol. 9, Part 1, pp. 42–53). Princeton University Press. (Original work published 1936)
Kihlstrom, J. F. (2007). The unconscious. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 783–806). John Wiley & Sons.
Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 71–93. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115154
Merikle, P. M., & Daneman, M. (1998). Psychological investigations of unconscious perception. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5(1), 5–18.
Meyen, S., Zerweck, I. A., Amado, C., von Luxburg, U., & Franz, V. H. (2020). Advancing research on unconscious priming: When can scientists claim an indirect task advantage? arXiv. https://arxiv.org/abs/2004.14987
Morena del Pozo, S., Laufs, H., Bonhomme, V., Laureys, S., & Tagliazucchi, E. (2020). Unconsciousness reconfigures modular brain network dynamics. arXiv.
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803–814. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803
Solms, M. (2018). The conscious id. Neuropsychoanalysis, 20(1), 3–20.
Weingarten, E., Chen, Q., McAdams, M., Yi, J., Hepler, J., & Albarracín, D. (2016). From primed concepts to action: A meta-analysis of the behavioral effects of incidentally presented words. Psychological Bulletin, 142(5), 472–497. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000030